School Start Times, Sleep, Behavioral, Health, and Academic Outcomes: a Review of the Literature

Epidemiologist, Division of Population Health, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 4770 Buford Hwy. NE, Mailstop F-78, Atlanta, GA 30341-3717, Phone: (770) 488-5362, Fax: (770) 488-5965, vog.cdc@9opi

Find articles by Anne G. Wheaton

Daniel P. Chapman

Epidemiologist, Division of Population Health, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 4770 Buford Hwy. NE, Mailstop F-78, Atlanta, GA 30341-3717, Phone: (770) 488-5463, vog.cdc@2cpd

Find articles by Daniel P. Chapman

Janet B. Croft

Branch Chief, Epidemiology and Surveillance Branch, Division of Population Health, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 4770 Buford Hwy. NE, Mailstop F-78, Atlanta, GA 30341-3717, Phone: (770) 488-2566, vog.cdc@0cbj

Find articles by Janet B. Croft

Anne G. Wheaton, Epidemiologist, Division of Population Health, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 4770 Buford Hwy. NE, Mailstop F-78, Atlanta, GA 30341-3717, Phone: (770) 488-5362, Fax: (770) 488-5965, vog.cdc@9opi;

Corresponding author. The publisher's final edited version of this article is available at J Sch Health

Abstract

BACKGROUND

Insufficient sleep in adolescents has been shown to be associated with a wide variety of adverse outcomes, from poor mental and physical health to behavioral problems and lower academic grades. However, most high school students do not get sufficient sleep. Delaying school start times for adolescents has been proposed as a policy change to address insufficient sleep in this population and potentially to improve students’ academic performance, reduce engagement in risk behaviors, and improve health.

METHODS

This paper reviews 38 reports examining the association between school start times, sleep, and other outcomes among adolescent students.

RESULTS

Most studies reviewed provide evidence that delaying school start time increases weeknight sleep duration among adolescents, primarily by delaying rise times. Most of the studies saw a significant increase in sleep duration even with relatively small delays in start times of half an hour or so. Later start times also generally correspond to improved attendance, less tardiness, less falling asleep in class, better grades, and fewer motor vehicle crashes.

CONCLUSIONS

Although additional research is necessary, research results that are already available should be disseminated to stakeholders to enable the development of evidence-based school policies.

Keywords: adolescents, school health, sleep, school start times, policy

BACKGROUND

Sleepy adolescents have doubtlessly been a problem for a long time. However, it is only since the late 1980s that this issue has progressed from teachers’ anecdotes of students falling asleep in class and parental complaints of daily struggles to get their children out of bed to scientific investigations into the causes and consequences of insufficient sleep.

Most adolescents may need at least 9 hours sleep per night 1–3 ; however, fewer than 8% of high school students report getting this amount. 4 Less than a third of students report 8 or more hours of sleep, and this proportion decreases as school grade level increases so that fewer than a quarter of high school seniors get this amount. 4 Healthy People 2020, a national initiative designed to guide disease prevention and health promotion efforts to improve the health of all Americans (http://www.healthypeople.gov/), contains 4 objectives related to sleep, including one for adolescents. 5 This objective is to “increase the proportion of students in grades 9 through 12 who get sufficient sleep (defined as 8 or more hours of sleep on an average school night).”

Insufficient sleep in children and adolescents has been shown to be associated with a wide variety of adverse outcomes in multiple aspects of their lives from poor mental and physical health to behavioral problems and poor academic grades. Insufficient sleep has been linked to excess weight, 6–13 decreased physical activity, 14 and increased food intake, possibly due to alterations in appetite-regulating hormones. 12 Results of investigations into longitudinal changes in weight attributable to sleep duration, however, have been mixed. 10,15

A solid body of literature has found that insufficient sleep in this young population is tied to poor mental health, including depression, depressive symptoms, 8,16–22 and suicidal ideation. 8,18,20,22–25 In addition, a few studies have shown an association between insufficient sleep and unhealthy risk behaviors including alcohol use, 17,19,20,22 tobacco smoking, 20,22 marijuana use, 20,22 use of other illicit/prescription drugs, 22 unhealthy weight control strategies, 26 and recent sexual activity. 20 Other factors that have been found to be associated with insufficient sleep include risk-taking behaviors, 27 bullying, 28 school violence-related behaviors, 29 and physical fighting. 20 Short sleep duration has also been found to be associated with a higher risk of unintentional injury. 30 Finally, students who do not get enough sleep also may be more likely to have problems paying attention and poor academic performance, 17,21,31–36 although not all research agrees. 37,38 One of these negative studies failed to find a correlation between school night sleep duration and grade point average. 37 However, class grading and subsequently grade point averages are not standardized and may vary by subject, teacher, and school. That study also did not adjust by sex of student, which was a strong predictor of grade point average. Ming et al. found that “students with a sleep length of less than 7 hours on both weekdays and weekends exhibited poorer performance, while those who made up this sleep loss on weekends did not.” 38 That study also relied on a non-standardized measure of academic performance and did not adjust for variables such as grade in school, which is strongly related to prevalence of insufficient sleep.

Adolescents tend to get insufficient sleep because of a combination of late bedtimes and early rise times. External factors that contribute to later bedtimes among adolescents include an increase in schoolwork; participation in afterschool activities, including employment; fewer parent-set bedtimes; and late-night use of technology in the bedroom. 39–43 Biology also plays a part in later bedtimes among adolescents. One of the early changes associated with puberty is alteration of a child’s circadian rhythms, such that adolescents are more alert in the afternoons and evenings and require morning sleep. 44 Their natural body clocks can keep adolescents awake until 11 PM or later, in spite of going to bed earlier and good sleep hygiene, such as avoiding stimulating activity at night and minimizing caffeine intake in the afternoon or evening. 39 School-based sleep promotion programs have been tried as a means of improving sleep hygiene among adolescents. However, these programs may improve knowledge of sleep without having a significant effect on behavior. 45,46

Rise times, on the other hand, are primarily determined by a single factor—school start times. 43 Delaying school start times for adolescents has been proposed as a policy change to address insufficient sleep in this population and potentially to improve students’ academic performance, reduce engagement in risk behaviors, and improve health. In 2014, the American Academy of Pediatrics published a policy statement urging middle and high schools to adjust start times to permit students to obtain adequate sleep and improve physical and mental health, safety, academic performance, and quality of life. 3 This paper reviews studies examining the association between school start times, sleep, and other outcomes including academic performance, mental health, and motor vehicle collisions among adolescent students. This paper, and especially the table provided, is intended to be a resource for educators, parents, and other stakeholders who wish to learn more about the impact of changing school start times for adolescents.

LITERATURE REVIEW

Methods

An outline of the steps in identification of articles and reports included in the literature review is presented in the Figure . To be included in the review, analyses had to include the variable of “school start time” either by comparing different schools with different start times (cross-sectional) or the same schools before and after changes in start times (longitudinal). Since the focus of this review is on the effect of school start times on adolescent students, studies that focused on elementary school (pre-middle school) students were excluded. Additionally, reports had to be available in English, but could include schools either in or outside the United States. Articles for the literature review were initially identified through a PubMed search for “school start time” OR “school start times” (Step A). Some exclusions from this initial search included a study that compared students in public or private schools to homeschooled students, 47 a comparison of adolescent sleep during summer and during the school year, 48 and one that modeled the effect of modifying school start times on the frequency of encounters between child pedestrians and motor vehicles. 49 Another broader PubMed search for “sleep” AND “adolescent” AND “school” resulted in approximately 3200 articles (Step B). Titles and abstracts were reviewed to identify studies that might meet primary inclusion criteria. Full-text review of candidate articles confirmed inclusion of articles for this review. Reference lists of articles identified in Steps A and B and reviews of the topic of school start times were reviewed for identification of additional reports (Step C). Several of the earliest studies with results presented in abstract form only were identified in this manner, 50–54 as were some studies from the education or economics literature that were not indexed by PubMed. 55–59 A Scopus search for “school start time” (all fields) found 320 documents (Step D), of which 3 satisfied the inclusion criteria and had not been identified in previous steps. Finally (Step E), one additional non-duplicative report was identified in the reference list for articles in Step D. Final searches in Steps A, B, and D were conducted July 1, 2015. For this review, 38 reports were reviewed for years of study, study design, sample size, students’ ages, location, school start times, outcomes, and key reported findings. Five of these reports are listed more than once in the table. Three reports included multiple study populations whose data are analyzed separately. 59–61 Three reports included cross-sectional and longitudinal components, with data for each component presented separately. 55,57,61

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc. Object name is nihms746414f1.jpg

Identification of Reports for Review

The primary limitation to this review is its reliance on indexing by PubMed and Scopus, which could lead to omission of reports not indexed by these databases. To address this limitation, we also examined the reference lists on previous articles on the topic of school start times (Steps C and E). Five of the 38 reports were identified in this manner. Publication bias and over-representation of studies with significant results is also possible, however, some studies included in this review had results that did not agree with the majority.

Types of School Start Time Studies

We reviewed 38 reports on the impact of changes in school start time (see Table ). Unfortunately, the earliest reports, 50–54 which have been cited frequently, were only published in abstract form. However, since they seem to have provided the impetus for much of the subsequent research and are fairly detailed, we have included them in this review. The studies presented in this review can be categorized as either cross-sectional or longitudinal. The cross-sectional studies simply compared characteristics for students at two or more schools having different start times, such that School A had an earlier start time than School B and compared how students at the two schools with respect to such outcomes as sleep duration, school attendance, academic performance, and morning traffic accidents among teens.

Table

Descriptions of School Start Time (SST) Studies in Preadolescents, Adolescents, and College-Age Students

Study
Author(s)(Year)
Year(s) of Study
Study DesignSample SizeAgeLocationSchool Start TimesOutcomesKey Findings
Carskadon et al. (1998) 62
Not specified
Longitudinal
(before vs after SST
change)
26
(same students)
Grades 9–10Rhode IslandYear 1–9th grade: 8:25
Year 2–10th grade: 7:20
Sleep schedules (sleep diaries and data
from electronic wrist monitors), multiple
sleep latency tests, polysomnography.
Earlier SST associated with earlier rise time (p .05), shorter sleep duration (p < .05), shorter REM
latency (p < .05), shorter sleep latency on multiple
sleep latency tests (p = .004), and more students
with REM sleep during multiple sleep latency tests
(p NR).
Epstein et al. (1998) 72
Not specified
Cross-sectional
(students in 18
schools)
572Grade 518 schools
throughout Israel
Early risers: 7:10 (2+
times/week)
Regular risers: 8:00
Sleep duration, bedtime, rise time
(weekdays and weekends), sleepiness,
daytime functioning, sleep difficulties.
Early risers reported shorter weekday sleep
duration (p = .0004), more complaints of feeling
tired throughout the day (p = .045), more daytime
sleepiness (p = .004), and more difficulty
concentrating and paying attention (p = .0001).
Wahlstrom et al. (1998) 60
Not specified
Cross-sectional
(3 districts)
NRGrades 10–12MinnesotaDistrict A (Minneapolis):
8:30
District B: 7:25
District C: 7:15
School Sleep Habits Survey 35 (study,
work, sleep, and school habits, and
preferences).
Later SST corresponded to later rise time (p .0001), same bedtime, and longer weeknight sleep
duration (p < .0001). Later SST also associated
with less daytime sleepiness (p < .0001), less
struggling to stay awake during academic tasks (p
< .01 for A vs C), fewer tardies (p < .001), less
falling asleep in morning classes (p <.005), fewer
depressive symptoms (p < .001 for A vs C), less
time spent at work during school week (p < .05),
and higher self-reported grades (p < .05).
Wahlstrom et al. (1998) 60
Not specified
Cross-sectional
(3 districts)
NRGrades 7–8MinnesotaDistrict A (Minneapolis):
7:35
District B: 8:00
District C: 8:00
School Sleep Habits Survey 35 (study,
work, sleep, and school habits, and
preferences).
Similar associations as in HS students. Later SST
corresponded to longer sleep duration (p < .05 for
A vs C), same bedtime, and later rise time (p < .05
for A vs C). Later SST also associated with less
daytime sleepiness (p < .05 for A vs B), less
struggling to stay awake during academic tasks (p
< .05 for A vs B), lower depression scores (p < .05
for A vs B), and less hours of homework (p < .05
for A vs B and C).
Wolfson & Carskadon (1998) 35
1994
Cross-sectional
(students in 4 schools
in 3 public school
districts)
3120Grades 9–12Rhode Island7:10 – 7:30School Sleep Habits Survey (study,
work, sleep, and school habits and
preferences)
Students at school with earliest SST (7:10) reported
earlier rise times than students at the other schools
(p < 0.001). Other sleep related differences did not
meet effect size criterion.
Baldus (2002) 63
Not specified
Longitudinal
(before vs after SST
change)
31
(same recruits,
2 sleep schedules)
US Navy
recruits
(mean 21.3
years)
Great Lakes,
Illinois
Early reveille: 9PM-5AM
sleep time
Late reveille: 10PM-6AM
sleep time
Sleep duration.Later reveille associated with longer sleep duration
(p = .01). The 10PM bedtime—6AM rise time
resulted in 22 more minutes of sleep per night per
recruit.
Wahlstrom (2002) 55
1994–2000
Longitudinal
(before vs after SST
change)
>12 000 (7 HS)Grades 9–12Minneapolis,
Minnesota
Pre-change: 7:15
Post-change: 8:40
Class grades, attendance, tardiness,
graduation rates, rates of continuous
enrollment.
After SST change, % continuously enrolled
increased (p < .05), attendance rates increased
(among students not continuously enrolled) (p .001), and letter grades increased slightly (not
significant).
Wahlstrom (2002) 55
1997–2001
Cross-sectional
(Minneapolis HS vs
District B)
467 (Minneapolis
HS)
169 (District B)
Grades 9–12Minneapolis,
Minnesota
Minneapolis HS: 8:40
District B: 7:30
School Sleep Habits Survey 35 (study,
work, sleep, and school habits and
preferences).
Later SST corresponded to longer sleep duration (p
< .001), same bedtime, and later rise time (p .001). Later SST also associated with less daytime
sleepiness (p < .01), less struggling to stay awake
during academic tasks (p < .01), fewer tardies (p .01)/absences (p < .05), less falling asleep in
morning classes (p < .001), and fewer depressive
symptoms (p < .05).
Dexter et al. (2003) 73
2002
Cross-sectional
(2 schools with
different SSTs)
193 (School 1)
587 (School 2)
Grades 10–11Altoona (1) and
Chippewa Falls
(2), Wisconsin
School 1: 7:50
School 2: 8:35
Weeknight sleep duration, Epworth
Sleepiness Scale 91 .
Mean weeknight sleep duration was longer in
School 2 compared to School 1 (p = .039).
Arlington Public Schools (2005) 81
2000–2002
Longitudinal (before
vs after SST change)
3 HS, 5 MSGrades 7–11Arlington County,
Virginia
HS: 7:30 (2000–2001),
8:15 (2001–2002)
MS: 8:10 (2000–2001),
7:50 (2001–2002)
Academic grades (1st period),
attendance, survey of students’
perceptions of how alert and prepared to
start school day, survey of teachers’
perceptions of student behavior. Surveys
were conducted in 2002 only.
Respondents were asked to recall prior to
SST changes.
10th grade students’ 1st period grades improved
after 45 minute SST delay (p < .001), but
attendance appeared to decrease. No significant
change in 7th grade students’ 1st period grades after
20 minute SST advance.
Adam et al. (2007) 68
2002–2003
Cross-sectional24545–z19 yearsNationally (USA)
representative
NRSleep duration, bedtime, rise time
(weekdays and weekends).
For older children (12–19 years), an hour later SST
was associated with 0.57 hour more weekday sleep
(p < .01), a 0.62 hour later rise time (p < .01), and
no difference in bedtime.
Wolfson et al. (2007) 64
2003–2004
Cross-sectional
(2 schools with
different SSTs)
205Grades 7–8Urban New
England school
district
Early school: 7:15
Late school: 8:37
School Sleep Habits Survey 35 (sleep
duration, bedtime, rise time—weekdays
and weekends, sleepiness, sleep-wake
behavior problems, sleep hygiene scale),
official academic performance,
attendance, tardiness.
Late SST associated with later bedtimes (in autumn
only) (p < .05), later rise times (p < .001), longer
sleep duration (65 minutes) (p < .001), less
weekend oversleep (in autumn only)(p < .001), less
daytime sleepiness in the autumn (p < .05), fewer
sleep-wake behavior problems in the spring (p .05), higher grades (8th grade only) (p < .01), and
fewer tardies (p < .001). Weekend sleep patterns
were similar.
Danner and Phillips (2008) 74
1998–1999
Longitudinal
(before vs after SST
change)
9966 (Year 1)
10 656 (Year 2).
Sample size
included HS and
MS students, but
results only
presented for HS
students.
Grades 6–12KentuckyYear 1 (Y1): 7:30 (HS)
Year 2 (Y2): 8:30 (HS)
Bedtime and rise time (weekdays and
weekends), number of naps, Epworth
Sleepiness Scale 91 , how time spent
before and after school, motor vehicle
crash rates among 17- and 18-year-old
students.
Compared to Year 1, in Year 2 students reported
longer average sleep duration (p < .001), less catch-
up sleep on weekend nights (p < .001), and lower
scores on the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (p < .001).
Crash rates in the study county decreased after the
SST delay (p < .01).
O’Malley and O’Malley (2008) 56
2001–2004
Longitudinal
(before vs after SST
change)
297 (pre-delay)
977 (post-delay)
Grades 9–12Wilton,
Connecticut
Pre-delay: 7:35
Post-delay: 8:15
Condensed School Sleep Habits
Questionnaire 35 .
Later SST corresponded to longer weeknight sleep
duration (p < .001), later rise time (p < .001), later
bedtime (p = .03), and fewer problems with
sleepiness (p < .001).
Owens, Belon, Moss (2010) 65
2008–2009
Longitudinal
(before vs after SST
change)
201 (mostly
boarders)
Grades 9–12Rhode IslandPre-change: 8:00
Post-change: 8:30
Bedtime and rise time (weekdays and
weekends), sleepiness-related behaviors,
health center visits, absences/tardies.
Delayed SST associated with longer school night
sleep durations (p < .001), earlier bedtimes (p .001), later rise times (p < .001), greater sleep
satisfaction (p < .001), less sleepiness (p < .001),
less sleepiness-related behaviors (p < .001), less
depressed mood (p < .001), fewer visits to health
center for fatigue-related symptoms (p = 0.03), and
fewer absences/tardies (p < .05).
Zhang et al. (2010) 66
Not specified
Cross-sectional4470 (mother-
father-child
community based
trios)
Mean age 9.2
± 1.8y
Hong KongMorning school: ~7:35
Whole-day school: ~8:08
Afternoon school:
~12:54pm
Bedtime, rise time, time-in-bed
(weekday, weekend, & long holidays),
napping
Students in morning schools (earliest SST) had
earliest bedtimes & rise times, shortest time-in-bed,
most weekend sleep compensation, and largest
proportion of daytime napping.
Carrell et al. (2011) 58
2004–2009
Cross-sectional6165U.S. Air Force
Academy
freshmen
Colorado Springs, ColoradoEarly (2004–2005, 2005–
2006): 7:00. Middle (2006–
2007): 7:30. Late (2007–
2008, 2008–2009): 7:50.
For each schedule, some
students had a first period
class while others did not.
Academic grades by scheduling
characteristics.
Students assigned to a first period course had
poorer grades; however, this association became
weaker and became statistically insignificant as the
start time moved from 7:00 AM to 7:50 AM.
Students performed worse in first period classes
compared to other periods, but those with first
period classes also performed worse in their
subsequent classes on that schedule day.
Hinrichs (2011) 59
1993–2002
Cross-sectional
(primarily, but with a
longitudinal
component)
196,617
observations (may
include repeat test
takers) (students
from 73 schools)
Grades 10–12Twin Cities
metropolitan area
(Minneapolis-St.
Paul, Minnesota)
7:15–8:40ACT scores, attendance ratesNo association between SST and ACT scores or
attendance rates.
Hinrichs (2011) 59
2000–2006
Cross-sectionalApproximately 400
public high schools
Grades 10–12Kansas (public
high schools in the
state)
Mean (SD): 7:59 (0:15)School-level test score data on Kansas
Mathematics Assessment, Reading
Assessment, Science Assessment, and
Social Studies Assessment.
No association between SST and scores on reading,
mathematics, science, and social studies
assessments.
Hinrichs (2011) 59
2000–2007
Cross-sectional75 schoolsGrades 9–12?
(not specified
in report, but
described as
high schools)
Virginia suburbs of
Washington D.C.
Scores on standardized end-of-course
exams.
No association between SST and test scores.
Lufi et al. (2011) 70
Not specified
Longitudinal
(before vs after SST
change—short term
change only)
47Grade 8IsraelEarly class: 7:30
Late class: 8:30
Sleep duration, bedtime, rise time, sleep
efficiency (data from electronic wrist
monitors), attention tests.
Later SST associated with longer sleep duration (p
< .01) and improved attention (p < .01).
Ming et al. (2011) 38
Not specified
Cross-sectional1941 (students
from 2 public high
schools, 1
vocational high
school, and two
extracurricular
high school
programs whose
students came from
various high
schools throughout
the state)
Grades 9–12New JerseyVaried from 7:00 to 8:45
For analyses, early SST
(before or at 7:30) vs late
SST (after 7:30)
Sleep duration, bedtime, rise time,
napping, perceived sleep adequacy, night
awakenings, prolonged sleep onset
Early SST associated with short weekday sleep
duration (receiving adequate night sleep (p < .0001), and
higher likelihood of afterschool naps (p < .0001)
and night awakenings (p =NR).
Vorona et al. (2011) 87
2007–2008
Cross-sectional
(2 cities with
different SSTs in
public schools)
NR16–18 yearsVirginia Beach and
Chesapeake,
Virginia
Early schools (Virginia
Beach): 7:20–7:25
Late schools (Chesapeake):
8:40–8:45
Motor vehicle crash rates in each city.Crash rates among 16-to 18-year-olds were higher
(p < .01) in Virginia Beach (which has earlier
SSTs).
Yilmaz et al. (2011) 75
2008
Cross-sectional344115–18 yearsGaziantep, TurkeyEarly: 7:00–8:00 (morning)
Late: 12:00–12:40 pm
(afternoon)
Bedtime, rise time, sleep latency
(weekdays & weekends), ideal night
sleep time, napping, sleep hygiene
Compared to those with afternoon SSTs, students
with morning SSTs reported shorter night time
sleep on weekdays and weekends (p < .001) and
shorter ideal night time sleep (p < .001). Early SST
students were more likely to report daytime
napping and needing help to awaken in the morning
(p < .001) and less likely to report a long sleep
latency (>30 minutes) on weekdays and weekends
(p < .001).
Vedaa et al (2012) 78
Not specified
Cross-sectional55 students
(intervention
school)
51 students
(control school)
Grade 10NorwayIntervention school: 9:30
(Mondays), 8:30 (rest of
week)
Control school: 8:30 (all
days)
Karolinska Sleepiness Scale 92 , reaction
time tests, Positive and Negative Affect
Schedule, sleep diary
Compared to control school students, students at
intervention school slept >1 hour longer on Sunday
night (p < .05), had a smaller difference between
Saturday and Sunday night sleep duration (p = .04),
and shorter sleep latency on Sunday night. Students
at the intervention school had fewer lapses on
reaction time tests (p = .02) and faster reaction
times (p = .02) on Monday than Friday compared
to the control students. No differences in sleepiness
or positive or negative affect.
Edwards (2012) 57
1999–2006
Cross-sectional
component
20,530 students
(1999–2000)
27,686 students
(2005–2006)
Grades 6–8Wake County,
North Carolina
7:30–8:45 (53% of students
start at 7:30, 22% start at
8:15)
End of year standardized test scores in
reading and math; time spent watching
television and doing homework;
absences
Later SST corresponded to higher standardized test
scores on both math and reading tests (1 hour later
corresponded to 3 percentile increase), less time
watching television, more time doing homework,
and fewer absences.
Edwards (2012) 57
1999–2006
Longitudinal
component (before vs
after SST change for
schools that changed
SST between 1999-
2006)
20,530 students
(1999–2000)
27,686 students
(2005–2006)
Grades 6–8Wake County,
North Carolina
7:30–8:45 (Some schools
started earlier and some
started later after SST
change)
End of year standardized test scores in
reading and math; time spent watching
television and doing homework
Later SST corresponded to higher standardized test
scores on both math and reading tests (1 hour later
corresponded to 2 percentile increase in math and
1.5 percentile increase in reading), less time
watching television, and more time doing
homework.
Borlase et al (2013) 69
1999–2008
Longitudinal (before
vs after SST change
in 2006)
212 students in
Grades 9 and 12
(1999)
455 students in
Grades 9, 11, and
12 (2008)
13–17 yearsWellington, New
Zealand
Senior students (Grade
12): 9:00 (pre), 10:30
(post)
Junior students (Grades 9,
11): 9:00 (no change)
School Sleep Habits Survey 35 (study,
work, sleep, and school habits, and
preferences), technology use, Epworth
Sleepiness Scale 91 , morningness-
eveningness preference, sleep/fatigue
problems
Grade 12 students after the SST delay (2008) had
longer sleep duration (p < .01), earlier bedtime (p .01), later rise time (p < .001), less sleep
discrepancy (sleep duration on school nights vs.
non-school nights)(p < .01), and less sleepiness (p
< .01) compared to Grade 12 students before the
delay or Grade 11 students (2008).
Li et al. (2013) 36
2007–2009
Cross-sectional/
Longitudinal (6
schools before vs
after SST change)
525 (baseline)
553 (2-year follow-
up)
Grades 4–5
(mean age
10.8y)
Shanghai, China2 control schools, no
change in SST: 7:30
2 schools, intervention 1:
7:30 (pre), 8:00 (post)
2 schools, intervention 2:
7:30 (pre), 8:30 (post)
Sleep duration, bedtime, rise time,
daytime sleepiness (Chinese version of
Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire)
At baseline, students at the 6 schools had similar
sleep characteristics. At follow-up, later SST
associated with later bedtime (p < .05), rise time (p
< .001), longer sleep duration (p < .001), and less
daytime sleepiness. Comparing baseline to follow-
up, mean sleep duration decreased for students at
control schools (no change in SST) but increased
for students at intervention schools.
Perkinson-Gloor et al. (2013) 34
2010–2011
Cross-sectional2373 (early)
343 (delayed start)
Grades 8–9Mid-size city in
northwestern
Switzerland
Early: not specified
Delayed: 20 minutes later
Sleep duration, bedtimes, rise times
(weekdays and weekends), daytime
sleepiness, behavioral persistence,
attitude towards life, school grades
(mathematics and German language).
Later SST associated with longer weeknight sleep
duration (p < .001), later weeknight bedtimes (p =
.04 boys, p < .001 girls), later weekday rise times
(p < .001), and less daytime sleepiness (p < .001
boys, p = .03 girls).
Short et al. (2013) 76
1997–2000 (U.S.)
2008–2010 (Australia)
Cross-sectional302 (U.S.)
385 (Australia)
Grades 9–12
(mean age
16.0y) (U.S.)
Years 9–11
(mean age
15.6y)
(Australia)
Rhode Island
(U.S.)
South Australia
(Australia)
U.S.: ~7:45
Australia: 8:20–9:00
School Sleep Habits Survey 35 (study,
work, sleep, and school habits and
preferences), 8-day sleep diary
School night sleep duration correlated with SST (r
= .29, p < .001). SST had largest effect on sleep
duration (compared with parent-set bedtimes and
extra-curricular load).
Boergers, Gable, and
Owens (2014) 67
2010–2011
Longitudinal (before
vs after SST change)
197 (boarding
students)
Grades 9–12Rhode IslandPre-delay: 8:00
Post-delay: 8:25
School Sleep Habits Survey 35 (study,
work, sleep, and school habits and
preferences), caffeine intake
questionnaire
After SST delay, mean wake times (school days)
were later (p < .001), mean school night sleep
duration was longer (p < .001), school night
bedtimes did not change. The percentage who slept
≥8 hour on school nights increased from 18% to
44% (p < .001). There were no changes in
nonschool night sleep patterns. After delay,
sleepiness scores (p < .001), depression scores (p .001), and caffeine use decreased (p < .05). No
changes in number of hours engaged in athletics,
extracurricular activities, and homework were
reported. No changes in self-reported grades were
reported. Weekday sleep duration decreased to
baseline after reverting to pre-delay SST.
Escribano & Diaz-Morales (2014) 77
Not specified
Cross-sectional
(students in 3
schools)
66912–16 yearsMadrid, Spain8:00, 8:15, 8:30Bedtimes, rise times, sleep duration,
attention task (at 3 times during school
day on 2 consecutive Wednesdays),
morningness/eveningness, inductive
reasoning
Average school night sleep was more than 30
minutes longer for 8:30 SST than 8:00 SST (p .001). Attention level highest for 8:15 SST on first
Wednesday (p < .001), but no difference by SST on
second Wednesday. Inductive reasoning score was
lowest for 8:30 SST (p < .01).
Chen et al. (2014) 79
2009
Cross-sectional4801 (students
from 12 senior HS
and 12 junior HS)
11–20 yearsShanghai, ChinaDichotimized: Before 7:00
vs. 7:00 or later
Adolescent Sleep Wake Scale 93 ,
bedtimes, wake-up times, sleep duration
(weekdays & weekends), parents’ sleep
habits
SST before 7:00 associated with sleeping (p = .015).
Vorona et al. (2014) 88
2009–2011
Cross-sectional
(2 counties with
different SST in
public schools)
16–18 yearsChesterfield
County, Virginia
and Henrico
County, Virginia
Early (Chesterfield): 7:20
Late (Henrico): 8:45
Weekday motor vehicle crash rates
during the school years (September-
May) 2009–2010 and 2010–2011.
In 2009–2010, crash rates among 16–18 year olds
were higher (p < .05) in Chesterfield County
(which has earlier SST). In 2010–2011, crash rates
among 16–17 year olds were higher (p < .05) in
Chesterfield County, but difference was not
statistically significant when 18 year olds were
included in comparison (p = .09). Adult crash rates
for the same time periods did not differ between the
two counties.
Milic et al. (2014) 80
2011
Cross-sectional821
(students from 4
HS)
15–19 yearsOsijek, CroatiaEarly: 7:00 AM or 1:00 PM
(alternating weeks)
Late: 8:00 AM or 2:00 PM
(alternating weeks)
Epworth Sleepiness Scale 91 ,
morningness/eveningness, napping,
academics (final semester grade)
Students with earlier SST performed better
academically and had earlier chronotypes (morning
preference) (p < .001). No difference in sleepiness.
Wahlstrom et al. (2014) 61
2011–2013
Cross-sectional9089Grades 9–125 HS in Minnesota,
2 HS in Colorado,
1 HS in Wyoming
After delay, SST varied
from 8:00 to 8:55
Sleep duration, bedtimes, rise times
(weekdays and weekends), sleep-related
tardiness, sleeping during class.
SST associated with percent of students sleeping
≥8 hours/school night (R 2 = .8878, schools starting before 8:30, 66% for school
starting at 8:55).
Wahlstrom et al. (2014) 61
2011–2013
Longitudinal
(before vs after SST
change)
446 (pre-delay)
459 (post-delay)
Grades 9–12Jackson Hole,
Wyoming
Pre-delay: 7:35
Post-delay: 8:55
Sleep duration, bedtimes, rise times
(weekdays and weekends).
Average school night sleep increased from 7.5h to
8.2h. Average weekend sleep decreased from 9.3h
to 9.0h.
Wahlstrom et al. (2014) 61
2004–2013
Longitudinal
(before vs after SST
change)
NRGrades 9–125 HS in Minnesota,
2 HS in Colorado,
1 HS in Wyoming
Before delay, SST varied
from 7:30 to 7:50
After delay, SST varied
from 8:00 to 8:55
Attendance, academic performance, car
crashes.
Most schools saw a decrease in tardiness and an
increase in GPA. Two out of the four areas for
which car crash data was available saw a major
decrease in car crashes involving 16- to 18-year-old
drivers (≥65%). One saw a small decrease (6%),
while another saw a small increase (9%).
Paksarian et al. (2015) 71
2001–2004
Cross-sectional7216 (245 schools)13–18 yearsNationally (USA)
representative
Mean: 8:01
Range: 7:05–9:22
Weeknight bedtime, sleep duration
(weeknight and weekend), weekend
compensatory sleep
Later SST associated with later bedtime (10 minute
later bedtime for each 30 minute delay in SST).
Later SST associated with longer sleep duration for
SST before 8:01 only (11 minutes for each 30
minute delay in SST). Difference only among boys
only and varied by urbanicity. Sleep duration
increased for boys in major metropolitan areas and
decreased for boys in nonurban counties. SST not
associated with weekend compensatory sleep.
Only Abstracts Available
Allen and Mirabile (1989) 50
Not specified
Cross-sectional61Grades 10–12
(Mean age
17.1 years)
NREarly school: 7:30
Late school: 8:00
Sleep duration, bedtimes, rise times
(weekdays and weekends).
Later SST corresponded to later rise times (p .05), but no difference in bedtimes. Sleep duration
difference not significantly different.
Allen (1992) 51
Not specified
Cross-sectional102Grade 12NREarly school: 7:40
Late schools: 8:30
Sleep duration, bedtimes, rise times
(weekdays and weekends).
Earlier SST corresponded to shorter weeknight
sleep duration (p < .035), more sleep problems (p =
.04), and later weekend rise times (p = .03).
Carskadon et al. (1995) 53
1994
Longitudinal (before
vs after SST change)
7Grade 9–10Rhode IslandLate (Grade 9): 8:25
Early (Grade 10): 7:20
Sleep duration, sleep start time, sleep
offset time (measured with actigraphy)
Earlier SST (65 minutes) corresponded to shorter
sleep duration (38 minutes, p < .02) due to earlier
rise times (59 minutes, p < .005). No significant
difference in school night sleep start time.
Kowalski and Allen 52
Not specified (1995)
Cross-sectional97 (early school)
119 (late school)
Grades 11–12NREarly school: 7:20
Late school: 9:30
Sleep duration, bedtimes, rise times
(weekdays and weekends).
Earlier SST corresponded to shorter weeknight
sleep duration (p = .002).
Wolfson et al. (1995) 54
1994
Longitudinal (before
vs after SST change)
15 (May have
included students
in Carskadon et al. 53 )
Grade 9–10Rhode IslandLate (Grade 9): 8:25
Early (Grade 10): 7:20
Sleep duration, sleep start time, sleep
efficiency (measured with actigraphy);
emotional/behavioral problems from
Youth Self Report (YSR) summary.
Earlier SST (65 minutes) corresponded to shorter
sleep duration (39 minutes, p < .001). No
significant difference in school night sleep start
time or sleep efficiency. YSR total problems and
externalizing scores decreased with earlier SST.

Abbreviations: ACT, originally American College Testing – one of two standardized tests commonly used in college admissions in the United States; HS, high school; MS, middle school; NR, not reported; SD, standard deviation; SST, school start time.

Changing school start times is often a major endeavor that involves coordination between school faculty and staff, transportation resources, parents, students, and administration, and can take years to accomplish. Cross-sectional studies can be conducted without having to wait for a school or school district to undertake a change in school start time. However, as with all cross- sectional studies, only associations between school start times and other variables can be shown—not cause and effect. Other school characteristics, such as socioeconomic status of students, can also have effects on sleep and other outcomes but is often not reported.

In the longitudinal studies, data are collected on students and schools before and after a change in start time. Some data that are regularly collected by schools, such as attendance and grades, can be obtained retroactively by researchers. Other data, such as those obtained from student sleep surveys, need to be collected before the start time change is implemented, and including these data can increase the duration of the study. Some studies evaluate characteristics of the same students before and after the change. Others evaluate the student population (either the entire school or specific grades in school) before and after the change. This second method, for instance, could compare outcomes in 9th-grade students in the year before the start time change with the same outcomes in 9th-grade students in the year after the change. Both of these methods have their drawbacks. Although it would seem to be preferable to evaluate the same students before and after a change, individual students can undergo significant changes from year to year that could exaggerate or diminish the effect of changing start times. However, by being able to evaluate changes in variables for individual students, researchers are able to address questions such as, “Did the students who got more sleep after the start time’s change have improved outcomes?” Although longitudinal studies may provide stronger evidence of causation than cross-sectional studies by addressing the issue of temporality, it is important to assess whether appropriate control conditions are used, whether analyses account for covariates (eg, age, sex, or socioeconomic status), and whether other explanatory variables are considered (eg, implementation of graduated drivers licensing).

Sleep and Sleepiness

Since delaying school start times is primarily intended to address the problem of insufficient sleep among adolescents, most of the studies focused on the association between school start times and sleep variables. Not surprisingly, students at schools with later start times got out of bed later on school days than those at earlier starting schools. 35,36,50,53,55,56,60,62–69 The association between school start times and bedtimes, however, was mixed. Of 19 studies that evaluated the association between school start times and bedtime, there were no start time-associated differences in weekday bedtimes in eleven studies. 35,50,53–55,60,62,67,68,70 However, six studies observed a later bedtime among students in schools that started at later times. 34,36,56,64,66,71 It should be noted that in the study by Wolfson and colleagues, this bedtime difference was observed only in the autumn. 64 Two studies unexpectedly reported earlier bedtimes after a delay in start times. 65,69 In the study by Owens et al., some students stated that after seeing the benefits of getting more sleep with the delay in school start times, they sought to further increase their sleep by going to bed earlier. 65 In Paksarian et al, bedtimes were delayed by only 10 minutes for each 30 minute delay in school start times. 71 In 29 reports, a later start time was found to be associated with longer weekday sleep duration, 34,36,38,51–56,60–79 including the studies that noted later bedtimes. 34,36,56,64,66,71 In contrast to the majority of studies that observed longer sleep duration in later starting schools, two studies comparing students from schools with different start times did not observe a significant difference in sleep duration. 35,50 For one of these, the difference in sleep duration did not meet the authors’ effect size criterion, although the difference would not have been expected to be large since the difference in school start times was only 20 minutes. 35 The other study observed longer sleep duration for students at the later starting school, but the difference was not statistically significant. 50 Another study observed a significantly longer sleep duration for 7th- and 8th-grade students of only one of two later-starting districts. 60 Sleep duration for the second later-starting district was also longer than for the early-starting district, but the difference was not statistically significant. 60 Paksarian et al. observed longer sleep durations with later school start times (11 minutes for each 30 minute delay in school start times), but only for schools that started before 8:01 AM. 71 In further analysis, the authors found the longer sleep duration for boys only (20 minutes for each 30 minute delay in school start times) and that there was variation by urbanicity, with an increase in sleep duration for boys in major metropolitan areas and a decrease for boys in nonurban counties. 71 The authors suggested that the different association according to urbanicity may be due to differences in mode of transportation to school and time spent traveling to school, but they did not have data to investigate the possible role of transportation in their study. Although nearly all the studies reviewed used self-reported sleep data either from sleep diaries or survey questions, the two studies that used data from electronic wrist monitors (actigraphs) worn during sleep confirmed the general finding of longer weeknight sleep duration for students with later start times. 62,70 Eleven of the positive studies found that students got at least one additional minute of sleep for every two minutes of difference in start time (eg, at least 30 minutes more sleep when start time was one hour later), 34,55,56,60,61,64,65,68,70,72,78 although six other studies observed smaller, yet statistically significant, differences. 51,52,62,63,73,74

In addition to reporting longer sleep durations, students with later start times were less likely to report daytime sleepiness or falling asleep in class in nearly all studies that assessed these outcomes. 34,36,55,56,60,64,65,69,72,74 Two studies observed no difference in sleepiness based on school start times. In the Norwegian study, 78 start times were delayed from 8:30 AM to 9:30 AM on Mondays only. The lack of association could have been due to either the change being in effect only one day a week or the earlier start time already being late enough. In the Croatian study, 80 the students at the earlier starting schools were predominantly boys, while girls made up most of the population at the later starting schools. That study found that girls had later chronotype (evening preference) and more sleepiness. In addition, the schools in the Croatian study alternated their schedule weekly, with schools starting in the morning one week and in the afternoon the next. Even with a delay in start time, falling asleep during class appears to remain a major problem, however. In Wahlstrom and colleagues’ survey of students post-start time change, 27% reported falling asleep in a morning class in the previous 2 weeks, and 29% fell asleep in an afternoon class. 61

Other methods for assessing students’ degree of sleep-deficiency were used in various studies. Students in a study by Carskadon and colleagues underwent polysomnography (a type of sleep study that measures multiple factors such as electrical activity in the brain, heart rate, movements of the eyelids and legs, and respiratory airflow) and multiple sleep latency tests. 62 After an advance in start time from 8:25 AM to 7:20 AM, students had a shorter REM sleep latency (time between sleep onset and onset of REM sleep) on polysomnography and a shorter sleep latency (time to sleep onset during a standard testing protocol). 62 Both of these results indicate sleep-deficiency. Differences in sleep patterns between weeknights and weekend nights can also indicate insufficient weeknight sleep as students try to make up for lost sleep on the weekends. Seven reports that evaluated weekend sleep patterns found more “catch-up” sleep on the weekends for earlier start times, 51,61,64,66,69,74,78 although one found no difference in this outcome. 71 One study assessed weeknight and weekend sleep and observed longer weeknight sleep duration and no change in weekend sleep patterns after a delay in school start time, which could be interpreted as a decrease in “catch-up” sleep. 67 However, the difference between weeknight and weekend sleep was not analyzed. Three studies that included information about daytime naps noted that students with earlier school start times reported more napping, 38,66,75 presumably in an attempt to make up for insufficient nighttime sleep. Sleepy adolescents may also attempt to lessen sleepiness with caffeine. One study asked students about caffeine consumption and observed that caffeine use decreased after a 25 minute delay in school start time. 67

Academics and Cognition

An outcome of particular interest to school administrators, teachers, and parents is academic performance; however, evaluating how delayed start times affect school grades or academic performance is difficult for several reasons. Class grading is not standardized and varies by subject, teacher, and school. Standardized tests such as the Scholastic Aptitude Test or the ACT are not taken by all students and are more likely to be taken by students planning to attend college. Finally, students with very good academic performance before a delay in school start time do not have much room for improvement. Given these limitations, however, some evidence suggests a positive association between later school start time and academic performance, 57,58,60,61,64,81 although the association may be relatively weak 55 and not universal. 59,67,80

Several studies have investigated the association between school start time and cognitive outcomes. Two studies found that students with later start times reported fewer problems concentrating and paying attention. 70,72 In contrast, a study in Spain measured attention level via a sustained attention task among students at three schools with different start times (8:00, 8:15, and 8:30) and observed the highest average attention level at the school starting at 8:15. 77 However, it should be noted that the mean inductive reasoning score, a measure of intelligence which is positively associated with attention, was significantly lower for the latest-starting school than the other two schools. 77 The Norwegian study that delayed start times on Mondays included reaction time tests and found that students at the school with delayed start time had significantly fewer lapses and faster reaction times on Monday than Friday compared to no difference among students at the control (no delay) school. 78

Several studies asked students how much time they spent on homework. There was no consistent association between school start time and homework time. Wahlstrom and colleagues found that students with later school start times reported less hours of homework, 60 whereas Edwards observed the reverse 57 and Boergers and colleagues saw no difference. 67 Interpretation of these results in difficult. An increase in time doing homework could indicate an improved ability to concentrate or less efficient studying. Since the value of homework is hotly debated, this outcome should not be taken out of context.

Attendance/Tardiness

School attendance is also important for academic success. A recent report found that short sleep duration was strongly associated with odds of school absences. 82 Several studies included in this review found that earlier start times were also related to more frequent tardiness and more absences. 55,57,60,61,64,65 In one study, even with delaying start times from 8:00 AM to 8:55 AM, nearly a third of students reported being late to class because of oversleeping in the 2 weeks before the survey. 61 However, one study of schools in the Minneapolis-St. Paul area found no association between school start times and overall attendance rates. 59 The report by Arlington Public Schools stated that “maturity, rather than starting time, has the biggest impact on attendance rates.” 81 However, when comparing 10 th graders before and after the delay in high school start times, attendance rates were lower after the delay. Despite this finding, academic performance improved for those students. The report also noted that the attendance reporting procedures changed during the course of the study.

Depression

Sleep is strongly linked with many psychiatric disorders, including depression and anxiety. 83 Although sleep problems may be symptoms of mental health disorders such as depression, there is also evidence of a causal relationship between insufficient sleep and depression, as well as mood in general. 84,85 Due to this observation, some school start time studies included depression symptoms as part of their student assessments. Students at later-starting schools appeared to experience fewer depression symptoms (lower depression scores). 55,60,65,67 Incidentally, shorter REM sleep latency, such as was observed by Carskadon and colleagues after an advance in start time, 62 is also often observed in major depression. 86 One study found no difference in students’ positive or negative affect with delayed school start time. 78 However, in that study, the start time was delayed on Mondays only.

Motor Vehicle Crashes

Other Outcomes

CONCLUSION

Delaying school start times for adolescents has been proposed as a policy change to address insufficient sleep among adolescents, a largely sleep-deprived population, and potentially to improve students’ academic performance, reduce engagement in risk behaviors, and improve health. Nearly all studies to date provide evidence that delaying school start time accomplishes the goal of increasing sleep duration among these students, primarily by delaying rise times. Most of the studies saw a significant increase in sleep duration even with relatively small delays in start times of half an hour or so. Later school start times also generally corresponded to improved attendance, less tardiness, less falling asleep in class, fewer depression symptoms, and fewer motor vehicle crashes. Although not all studies found that later start times corresponded to improved academic performance, no studies found a negative impact of later school start times on academics.

IMPLICATIONS FOR SCHOOL HEALTH

In 2014, the American Academy of Pediatrics published a policy statement urging middle and high schools to adjust start times to permit students to obtain adequate sleep and improve physical and mental health, safety, academic performance, and quality of life, and suggested that middle and high schools not start before 8:30 AM. 3 Schools and school districts cannot make evidence-based policy decision without data. Therefore, research results such as those presented in this review, as well as the recent recommendations by the American Academy of Pediatrics, should be disseminated to school districts, teachers, parents, and other stakeholders. The field still needs rigorous research, including trials with controls, if possible. Many questions remain, such as the issue of how late is late enough? Much of the focus has been on high school students, but biological changes begin earlier, so further research into middle-school students is warranted. More qualitative research about overcoming obstacles to delaying school start times would also be valuable.

Schools contemplating a change in school start time may consider partnering with researchers before a decision is even made. Baseline data on student sleep characteristics, tardiness due to sleepiness, and prevalence of falling asleep during class may be used in the initial decision of whether to delay school start times. Parents and administrators may not realize the extent of the problem of insufficient sleep among their students and this data may help persuade them that some action is necessary. If school start times are delayed, everyone would benefit from collection of detailed data before and after a time change. Not only would other schools or school districts contemplating a change benefit from expanded evidence, the district that undertook the start time change would be able to evaluate the impact of the change and communicate their findings to their stakeholders. Data collected should include not only sleep-related variables and academic achievement measures, but also measures of mental health (such as depression and anxiety symptoms), behavioral problems, risk behaviors, safety statistics such as motor vehicle crash rates and pedestrian injuries, and information on mode of transportation and travel time. Several obstacles to implementing start time delays are often cited, including costs of changing bus schedules, possible impact on athletics and extracurricular activities, and school faculty and staff resistant to change. However, as recently reported by Owens et al. 90 in their examination of school districts that have delayed school start times, many anticipated problems fail to materialize or are only temporary. Several school districts have seen savings in transportation costs after changes made to facilitate delayed start times. 90 Success stories describing how districts creatively overcame obstacles to school start time changes should be shared (for example http://www.startschoollater.net/success-stories.html) to provide ideas to other districts contemplating change.

Footnotes

Disclaimer: The findings and conclusions in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official position of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

Contributor Information

Anne G. Wheaton, Epidemiologist, Division of Population Health, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 4770 Buford Hwy. NE, Mailstop F-78, Atlanta, GA 30341-3717, Phone: (770) 488-5362, Fax: (770) 488-5965, vog.cdc@9opi.

Daniel P. Chapman, Epidemiologist, Division of Population Health, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 4770 Buford Hwy. NE, Mailstop F-78, Atlanta, GA 30341-3717, Phone: (770) 488-5463, vog.cdc@2cpd.

Janet B. Croft, Branch Chief, Epidemiology and Surveillance Branch, Division of Population Health, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 4770 Buford Hwy. NE, Mailstop F-78, Atlanta, GA 30341-3717, Phone: (770) 488-2566, vog.cdc@0cbj.

REFERENCES

1. Carskadon MA, Vieira C, Acebo C. Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep. 1993; 16 (3):258–262. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

2. Mercer PW, Merritt SL, Cowell JM. Differences in reported sleep need among adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 1998; 23 (5):259–263. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

3. Adolescent Sleep Working Group, Committee on Adolescence, Council on School Health. School start times for adolescents. Pediatrics. 2014; 134 (3):642–649. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

4. Eaton DK, McKnight-Eily LR, Lowry R, Perry GS, Presley-Cantrell L, Croft JB. Prevalence of insufficient, borderline, and optimal hours of sleep among high school students - United States, 2007. J Adolesc Health. 2010; 46 (4):399–401. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

5. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Office of Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. [Accessed March 26, 2014]; Healthy People. 2020 Available at: http://www.healthypeople.gov/2020/topicsobjectives2020/objectiveslist.aspx?topicId=38.

6. Lowry R, Eaton DK, Foti K, McKnight-Eily L, Perry G, Galuska DA. Association of sleep duration with obesity among US high school students. J Obes. 2012; 2012 :476914. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Lytle LA, Pasch KE, Farbakhsh K. The relationship between sleep and weight in a sample of adolescents. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2011; 19 (2):324–331. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

8. Do YK, Shin E, Bautista MA, Foo K. The associations between self-reported sleep duration and adolescent health outcomes: what is the role of time spent on Internet use? Sleep Med. 2013; 14 (2):195–200. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Shan XY, Xi B, Cheng H, Hou DQ, Wang Y, Mi J. Prevalence and behavioral risk factors of overweight and obesity among children aged 2–18 in Beijing, China. Int J Pediatr Obes. 2010; 5 (5):383–389. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. O’Dea JA, Dibley MJ, Rankin NM. Low sleep and low socioeconomic status predict high body mass index: a 4-year longitudinal study of Australian schoolchildren. Pediatr Obes. 2012; 7 (4):295–303. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. Morley BC, Scully ML, Niven PH, et al. What factors are associated with excess body weight in Australian secondary school students? Med J Aust. 2012; 196 (3):189–192. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. Hart CN, Carskadon MA, Considine RV, et al. Changes in children’s sleep duration on food intake, weight, and leptin. Pediatrics. 2013; 132 (6):e1473–e1480. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

13. Hjorth MF, Chaput JP, Ritz C, et al. Fatness predicts decreased physical activity and increased sedentary time, but not vice versa: support from a longitudinal study in 8- to 11-year-old children. Int J Obes (Lond) 2013 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. Foti KE, Eaton DK, Lowry R, McKnight-Ely LR. Sufficient sleep, physical activity, and sedentary behaviors. Am J Prev Med. 2011; 41 (6):596–602. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Lytle LA, Murray DM, Laska MN, Pasch KE, Anderson SE, Farbakhsh K. Examining the longitudinal relationship between change in sleep and obesity risk in adolescents. Health Educ Behav. 2013; 40 (3):362–370. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Short MA, Gradisar M, Lack LC, Wright HR, Dohnt H. The sleep patterns and well-being of Australian adolescents. J Adolesc. 2013; 36 (1):103–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. Pallesen S, Saxvig IW, Molde H, Sorensen E, Wilhelmsen-Langeland A, Bjorvatn B. Brief report: behaviorally induced insufficient sleep syndrome in older adolescents: prevalence and correlates. J Adolesc. 2011; 34 (2):391–395. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

18. Gangwisch JE, Babiss LA, Malaspina D, Turner JB, Zammit GK, Posner K. Earlier parental set bedtimes as a protective factor against depression and suicidal ideation. Sleep. 2010; 33 (1):97–106. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

19. Pasch KE, Laska MN, Lytle LA, Moe SG. Adolescent sleep, risk behaviors, and depressive symptoms: are they linked? Am J Health Behav. 2010; 34 (2):237–248. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. McKnight-Eily LR, Eaton DK, Lowry R, Croft JB, Presley-Cantrell L, Perry GS. Relationships between hours of sleep and health-risk behaviors in US adolescent students. Prev Med. 2011; 53 (4–5):271–273. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

21. Fredriksen K, Rhodes J, Reddy R, Way N. Sleepless in Chicago: tracking the effects of adolescent sleep loss during the middle school years. Child Dev. 2004; 75 (1):84–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Winsler A, Deutsch A, Vorona RD, Payne PA, Szklo-Coxe M. Sleepless in Fairfax: the difference one more hour of sleep can make for teen hopelessness, suicidal ideation, and substance use. J Youth Adolesc. 2015; 44 (2):362–378. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Jang SI, Lee KS, Park EC. Relationship between current sleep duration and past suicidal ideation or attempt among Korean adolescents. J Prev Med Public Health. 2013; 46 (6):329–335. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Fitzgerald CT, Messias E, Buysse DJ. Teen sleep and suicidality: results from the Youth Risk Behavior Surveys of 2007 and 2009. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011; 7 (4):351–356. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

25. Liu X. Sleep and adolescent suicidal behavior. Sleep. 2004; 27 (7):1351–1358. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. Wheaton AG, Perry GS, Chapman DP, Croft JB. Self-reported sleep duration, weight-control strategies among U.S. high school students. Sleep. 2013; 36 (8):1139–1145. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

27. O’Brien EM, Mindell JA. Sleep and risk-taking behavior in adolescents. Behav Sleep Med. 2005; 3 (3):113–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

28. Kubiszewski V, Fontaine R, Potard C, Gimenes G. Bullying, sleep/wake patterns and subjective sleep disorders: Findings from a cross-sectional survey. Chronobiol Int. 2014 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

29. Hildenbrand AK, Daly BP, Nicholls E, Brooks-Holliday S, Kloss JD. Increased risk for school violence-related behaviors among adolescents with insufficient sleep. J Sch Health. 2013; 83 (6):408–414. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

30. Lam LT, Yang L. Short duration of sleep and unintentional injuries among adolescents in China. Am J Epidemiol. 2007; 166 (9):1053–1058. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

31. Perez-Lloret S, Videla AJ, Richaudeau A, et al. A multi-step pathway connecting short sleep duration to daytime somnolence, reduced attention, and poor academic performance: an exploratory cross-sectional study in teenagers. J Clin Sleep Med. 2013; 9 (5):469–473. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

32. Miller NL, Shattuck LG, Matsangas P, Dyche J. Sleep and academic performance in U.S. military training and education programs. Mind Brain Educ. 2008; 2 (1):29–33. [Google Scholar]

33. Meijer AM. Chronic sleep reduction, functioning at school and school achievement in preadolescents. J Sleep Res. 2008; 17 (4):395–405. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

34. Perkinson-Gloor N, Lemola S, Grob A. Sleep duration, positive attitude toward life, and academic achievement: the role of daytime tiredness, behavioral persistence, and school start times. J Adolesc. 2013; 36 (2):311–318. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

35. Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents. Child Dev. 1998; 69 (4):875–887. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

36. Li S, Arguelles L, Jiang F, et al. Sleep, school performance, and a school-based intervention among school-aged children: a sleep series study in China. PLoS One. 2013; 8 (7):e67928. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

37. Eliasson A, Eliasson A, King J, Gould B, Eliasson A. Association of sleep and academic performance. Sleep Breath. 2002; 6 (1):45–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

38. Ming X, Koransky R, Kang V, Buchman S, Sarris CE, Wagner GC. Sleep insufficiency, sleep health problems and performance in high school students. Clin Med Insights Circ Respir Pulm Med. 2011; 5 :71–79. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

39. Carskadon MA. Factors influencing sleep patterns of adolescents. In: Carskadon MA, editor. Adolescent Sleep Patterns: Biological, Social and Psychological Influences. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2002. pp. 4–26. [Google Scholar]

40. Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med. 2010; 11 (8):735–742. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

41. Calamaro CJ, Mason TB, Ratcliffe SJ. Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics. 2009; 123 (6):e1005–e1010. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

42. Short MA, Gradisar M, Wright H, Lack LC, Dohnt H, Carskadon MA. Time for bed: parent-set bedtimes associated with improved sleep and daytime functioning in adolescents. Sleep. 2011; 34 (6):797–800. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

43. Knutson KL, Lauderdale DS. Sociodemographic and behavioral predictors of bed time and wake time among US adolescents aged 15 to 17 years. J Pediatr. 2009; 154 (3):426–430. 430 e421. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

44. Crowley SJ, Acebo C, Carskadon MA. Sleep, circadian rhythms, and delayed phase in adolescence. Sleep Med. 2007; 8 (6):602–612. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

45. Cassoff J, Knauper B, Michaelsen S, Gruber R. School-based sleep promotion programs: effectiveness, feasibility and insights for future research. Sleep Med Rev. 2013; 17 (3):207–214. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

46. Blunden SL, Chapman J, Rigney GA. Are sleep education programs successful? The case for improved and consistent research efforts. Sleep Med Rev. 2012; 16 (4):355–370. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

47. Meltzer LJ, Shaheed K, Ambler D. Start later, sleep later: school start simes and adolescent sleep in homeschool versus public/private school students. Behav Sleep Med. 2014:1–15. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

48. Hansen M, Janssen I, Schiff A, Zee PC, Dubocovich ML. The impact of school daily schedule on adolescent sleep. Pediatrics. 2005; 115 (6):1555–1561. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

49. Yiannakoulias N, Bland W, Scott DM. Altering school attendance times to prevent child pedestrian injuries. Traffic Inj Prev. 2013; 14 (4):405–412. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

50. Allen RP, Mirabile J. Self-reported sleep-wake patterns for students during the school year from two different senior high schools. Sleep Res. 1989; 21 :114. [Google Scholar]

51. Allen RP. Social factors associated with the amount of school week sleep lag for seniors in a early starting suburban high school. Sleep Res. 1992; 21 :114. [Google Scholar]

52. Kowalski NA, Allen RP. School sleep lag is less but persists with a very late starting high school. Sleep Res. 1995; 24 :124. [Google Scholar]

53. Carskadon MA, Wolfson AR, Tzischinsky O, Acebo C. Early school schedules modify adolescent sleepiness. Sleep Res. 1995; 24 :92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

54. Wolfson AR, Tzischinsky O, Brown C, Darley C, Acebo C, Carskadon MA. Sleep, behavior, and stress at the transition to senior high school. Sleep Res. 1995; 24 :115. [Google Scholar]

55. Wahlstrom K. Changing times: findings from the first longitudinal study of later high school start times. NASSP Bulletin. 2002; 86 (633):3–21. [Google Scholar]

56. O’Malley EB, O’Malley MB. Sleep and Psychiatric Disorders in Children and Adolescents. New York: Information Healthcare Publisher; 2008. School start time and its impact on learning and behavior; pp. 28–72. [Google Scholar]

57. Edwards F. Early to rise? The effect of daily start times on academic performance. Econ Educ Rev. 2012; 31 :970–983. [Google Scholar]

58. Carrell SE, Maghakian T, West JE. A’s from Zzzz’s? The causal effect of school start time on the academic achievement of adolescents. Am Econ J: Econ Policy. 2011; 3 (3):62–81. [Google Scholar]

59. Hinrichs P. When the bell tolls: the effects of school starting times on academic achievement. Educ Financ Policy. 2011; 6 (4):486–507. [Google Scholar]

60. Wahlstrom KL, Hendrix V, Frederickson J. School Start Time Study. Technical Report, Volume II: Analysis of Student Survey Data. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota, Center for Applied Research and Educational Improvement; 1998. [Google Scholar]

61. Wahlstrom K, Dretzke B, Gordon M, Peterson K, Edwards K, Gdula J. Center for Applied Research and Educational Improvement. St Paul, MN: University of Minnesota; 2014. Examining the Impact of Later School Start Times on the Health and Academic Performance of High School Students: a Multi-site Study. [Google Scholar]

62. Carskadon MA, Wolfson AR, Acebo C, Tzischinsky O, Seifer R. Adolescent sleep patterns, circadian timing, and sleepiness at a transition to early school days. Sleep. 1998; 21 (8):871–881. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

63. Baldus BR. Sleep Patterns in U.S. Navy recruits: an Assessment of the Impact of Changing Sleep Regimens. Monterey, CA: Naval Postgraduate School; 2002. [Google Scholar]

64. Wolfson AR, Spaulding NL, Dandrow C, Baroni EM. Middle school start times: the importance of a good night’s sleep for young adolescents. Behav Sleep Med. 2007; 5 (3):194–209. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

65. Owens JA, Belon K, Moss P. Impact of delaying school start time on adolescent sleep, mood, and behavior. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010; 164 (7):608–614. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

66. Zhang J, Li AM, Fok TF, Wing YK. Roles of parental sleep/wake patterns, socioeconomic status, and daytime activities in the sleep/wake patterns of children. J Pediatr. 2010; 156 (4):606–612. e605. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

67. Boergers J, Gable CJ, Owens JA. Later school start time is associated with improved sleep and daytime functioning in adolescents. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2014; 35 (1):11–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

68. Adam EK, Snell EK, Pendry P. Sleep timing and quantity in ecological and family context: a nationally representative time-diary study. J Fam Psychol. 2007; 21 (1):4–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

69. Borlase BJ, Gander PH, Gibson RH. Effects of school start times and technology use on teenagers’ sleep: 1999–2008. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2013; 11 (1):46–54. [Google Scholar]

70. Lufi D, Tzischinsky O, Hadar S. Delaying school starting time by one hour: some effects on attention levels in adolescents. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011; 7 (2):137–143. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

71. Paksarian D, Rudolph KE, He JP, Merikangas KR. School start time and adolescent sleep patterns: results from the US National Comorbidity Survey-Adolescent Supplement. Am J Public Health. 2015; 105 (7):1351–1357. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

72. Epstein R, Chillag N, Lavie P. Starting times of school: effects on daytime functioning of fifth-grade children in Israel. Sleep. 1998; 21 (3):250–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

73. Dexter D, Bijwadia J, Schilling D, Applebaugh G. Sleep, sleepiness and school start times: a preliminary study. WMJ. 2003; 102 (1):44–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

74. Danner F, Phillips B. Adolescent sleep, school start times, and teen motor vehicle crashes. J Clin Sleep Med. 2008; 4 (6):533–535. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

75. Yilmaz K, Kilincaslan A, Aydin N, Kul S. Understanding sleep habits and associated factors can help to improve sleep in high school adolescents. Turk J Pediatr. 2011; 53 (4):430–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

76. Short MA, Gradisar M, Lack LC, et al. A cross-cultural comparison of sleep duration between US And Australian adolescents: the effect of school start time, parent-set bedtimes, and extracurricular load. Health Educ Behav. 2013; 40 (3):323–330. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

77. Escribano C, Diaz-Morales JF. Daily fluctuations in attention at school considering starting time and chronotype: an exploratory study. Chronobiol Int. 2014; 31 (6):761–769. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

78. Vedaa Ø, Saxvig IW, Wilhelmsen-Langeland A, Bjorvatn B, Pallesen S. School start time, sleepiness and functioning in Norwegian adolescents. Scand J Educ Res. 2012; 56 (1):55–67. [Google Scholar]

79. Chen T, Wu Z, Shen Z, Zhang J, Shen X, Li S. Sleep duration in Chinese adolescents: biological, environmental, and behavioral predictors. Sleep Med. 2014 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

80. Milić J, Kvolik A, Ivković M, et al. Are there differences in students’ school success, biorhythm, and daytime sleepiness depending on their school starting times? Coll Antropol. 2014; 38 (3):889–894. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

81. Arlington Public Schools Office of Planning and Evaluation. [Accessed June 30, 2015]; Impact of 2001 Adjustments to High School and Middle School Start Times. 2005 Available at: http://www.fcps.edu/fts/taskforce07/documents/arlington605.pdf.

82. Hysing M, Haugland S, Stormark KM, Boe T, Sivertsen B. Sleep and school attendance in adolescence: Results from a large population-based study. Scand J Public Health. 2015; 43 (1):2–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

83. Sutton EL. Psychiatric disorders and sleep issues. Med Clin North Am. 2014; 98 (5):1123–1143. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0025712514000972. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

84. Wong ML, Lau EY, Wan JH, Cheung SF, Hui CH, Mok DS. The interplay between sleep and mood in predicting academic functioning, physical health and psychological health: a longitudinal study. J Psychosom Res. 2013; 74 (4):271–277. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

85. Wiebe ST, Cassoff J, Gruber R. Sleep patterns and the risk for unipolar depression: a review. Nat Sci Sleep. 2012; 4 :63–71. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

86. Benca RM. Mood disorders. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 4th. Philadelphia: Elsevier/Saunders; 2005. pp. 1311–1326. [Google Scholar]

87. Vorona RD, Szklo-Coxe M, Wu A, Dubik M, Zhao Y, Ware JC. Dissimilar teen crash rates in two neighboring southeastern Virginia cities with different high school start times. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011; 7 (2):145–151. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

88. Vorona RD, Szklo-Coxe M, Lamichhane R, Ware JC, McNallen A, Leszczyszyn D. Adolescent crash rates and school start times in two central Virginia counties, 2009–2011: a follow-up study to a southeastern Virginia study, 2007–2008. J Clin Sleep Med. 2014; 10 (11):1169–1177. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

89. Staff J, Schulenberg JE, Bachman JG. Adolescent work intensity, school performance, and academic engagement. Sociol Educ. 2010; 83 (3):183–200. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

90. Owens J, Drobnich D, Baylor A, Lewin D. School start time change: an in-depth examination of school districts in the United States. [Accessed July 6, 2015]; Mind Brain Educ. 2014 8 (4):182–213. Available at: http://www.startschoollater.net/uploads/9/7/9/6/9796500/schoolstarttimechange_mbe_owensdrobnichlewinbaylor_2014.pdf. [Google Scholar]

91. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1991; 14 (6):540–545. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

92. Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci. 1990; 52 (1–2):29–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

93. LeBourgeois MK, Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Wolfson AR, Harsh J. The relationship between reported sleep quality and sleep hygiene in Italian and American adolescents. Pediatrics. 2005; 115 (1 Suppl):257–265. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]